Saturday, June 22, 2013

Critical History

Critical histories can be applied to policies, practices, or institutions (among other phenomena). They can be constructed from archival and textual data, and/or from microhistories (a focus on a single person or place). Critical histories require the researcher to apply a critical theoretical framework (such as CRT, feminism, critical democracy, etc.) to analyze the data collected.



Examples:
Gilroy, P. & McNamara, O. (2009). A critical history of research assessment in the UK and its post-1992 impact on education. Journal of Education for Teaching: International Research and Pedagogy, 35(4), 321-335.

Reid, D.K. & Knight, M.G. (2006). Disability justifies exclusion of minority students: A critical history grounded in disability studies. Educational Researcher, 35(6), 18-23.

Friday, June 14, 2013

Levels of Evidence

It's useful to consider the varying levels of evidence when thinking about the strength of research. The levels in the figure below come from the medical literature - and tend to privilege a positivist research perspective. Notice that the methodological design of the research plays a role in its level - and  more generalizable, quantitative results are privileged. This is one perspective, and it is contested, especially in the social sciences!
From Pinto, L., Spares, S. & Driscoll, L. (2012). 95 Strategies for Remodeling Instruction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Saturation

Conclusions in qualitative research are drawn from patterns a researcher identifies in the data, or conclusions can uncover conceptual (NOT statistical) relationships. As such, we look for points of saturation to know when we have collected and analyzed enough data – and this can determine the sample size and the point at which analysis should end.

Data saturation is the point where new data and theirsorting only confirm the categories (often numbering between three and six orso), themes, and conclusions already reached. Onwuegbuzie and Leech (2007) also discuss theoretical saturation (Strauss & Corbin, 1990), and informational redundancy (Lincoln & Guba, 1985) as specific areas of saturation. There are various strategies for determining when saturation is reached, but researchers should consider a codebook to track themes and findings.

For more information, see the links above, and
Onwuegbuzie, A., & Leech, N. L. (2007). A Call for Qualitative Power Analyses. Quality & Quantity, 41,105–121. DOI 10.1007/s11135-005-1098-1

Some Sampling Methods Summarized

This is a general overview - please refer to other sources for details. The summary here is based on:

Onwuegbuzie, A., & Leech, N. L. (2007). A Call for Qualitative Power Analyses. Quality & Quantity, 41,105–121. DOI 10.1007/s11135-005-1098-1
 
Random Sampling

Sampling type
Description
Notes
Simple Random Sampling
Participants selected so every person in the population has the same chance of selection, and selection is independent (the selection of 1 doesn’t affect the selection of others). Random means that each person in the population is assigned a number, and the selection is based on a random table of numbers. This is different from the “conversational” use of the term random.
 
Requires an accurate list of the entire population – so if the population was “science teachers in the GTA,” the researcher would have to have a list of ALL science teachers including contact information, and draw from that
Stratified Random Sampling
Similar to above, but population is divided into homogenous sub-populations (e.g., males and females) and the sub-populations are randomized and selected
See above
Cluster Random Sampling
As above, but groups or clusters are randomly selected
As above
Systematic Random Sampling
As above, but the researcher selects every kth sampling frame member,
where k represents the population size divided by the desired sample size
As above
Multi-stage Random Sampling
The researcher samples in two or more stages because either the population is relatively large or its members cannot easily be identified
 

 

 

Non-Random Sampling

Sampling type
Description
Notes
Purposive Maximum Variation Sampling
A wide range of individuals, groups, or settings is purposively selected so that different and divergent perspectives are represented
The researcher has to have expert knowledge of and access to the population
Purposive Homogeneous Sampling
sampling individuals, groups, or settings because they all possess
similar characteristics or attributes
 
Purposive Critical Case Sampling
individuals, groups, or settings are selected that
bring to the fore the phenomenon of interest such that the researcher can
learn more about the phenomenon than would have been learned without including these critical cases
As above
Theory-based sampling
individuals, groups, or settings are selected because they help the researcher to develop or expand a theory
 
Confirming and Disconfirming Case Samples
This is often applied at the end of data collection based on what the individual cases said
Occurs at the end of the research process, in combination with another sampling method
Snowball Sampling
Asking participants who have already been selected for the study to recruit
other participants.
Occurs during data collection
Extreme Case Samples
an outlying case or one with
more extreme characteristics is studied
 
Intensity Sampling
researcher studies individuals, groups, or settings that experience the phenomenon intensely but not extremely
 
Typical Case Sampling
researcher should consult several experts in the field of study in order to obtain a consensus as to what example(s) is typical of the phenomenon
Requires access to “experts” for consensus
Politically Important Sampling
researcher selects informants to be included/excluded because they connect with politically sensitive issue
 
Random Purposeful Sampling
the researcher chooses cases at random (see above for clarification on the formal definition of “random”) from the sampling frame consisting of a purposefully selected sample. The researcher first obtains a list of individuals of interest for study using one of the 15 other methods of purposeful sampling, and then randomly selects cases
 
Stratified Purposeful Sampling
As above, but the selection is stratified (see above for the definition of stratified)
 
Criterion Sampling
individuals, groups, or settings are selected that meet criteria central to the research
 
Opportunistic Sampling
the researcher capitalizes on opportunities during data collection stage to select cases. Cases could represent typical, negative, critical, or extreme cases
 
Mixed Purposeful Sampling
mixing of more than one sampling
strategy (e.g., one extreme case sample and another critical case sample). Results can be compared to
triangulate data
 
Convenience Sampling
selecting individuals or groups that happen to be available and are willing to participate at the time
 
Quota Sampling
Cases selected based on specific characteristics and quotas
A main limitation is that only those accessible at the time of selection have a chance of being selected

 

 

 

 

 

How many? Sampling in qualitative research


Onwuegbuzie and Leech (2007) stress that though qualitative research typically relies on small samples, the sample size is important because it determines the extent to which the researcher can make generalizations. Sample sizes in qualitative research should small enough so that the researcher can extract thick, rich data, but also large enough that saturation (data, theoretical saturation, and informational redundancy) are achieved (Lincoln & Guba, 1985; Onwuegbuzie & Leech, 2007).

Mason (2010) cites Guest, Bunce And Johnson ‘s (2006) finding that only 7 sources provided guidelines for qualitative sample sizes. They are:

Source
Methodology
Sample Size
Morse, J.M. (1994). Designing funded qualitative research. In Norman K. Denzin & Yvonna S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed., pp.220-35). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Ethnography and ethnoscience
30-50 interviews
Bernard, H.R. (2000). Social research methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Ethnography and ethnoscience
30-60 interviews
Creswell, J. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Grounded theory
20-30 interviews
Morse (1994)
Grounded theory
30-50 interviews
Cresswell (1998)
Phenomenology
5-25 interviews
Morse (1994)
Phenomenology
At least 6 interviews
Bertaux, D. (1981). From the life-history approach to the transformation of sociological practice. In D. Bertaux (Ed.), Biography and society: The life history approach in the social sciences (pp.29-45). London: Sage.
All qualitative
At lease 15

Sources cited:
Guest, G., Bunce, A., & Johnson, L. (2006). "Howmany interviews are enough? An experiment with data saturation andvariability". Field Methods, 18(1), 59-82.

Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Mason, M. (2010). Sample Size and Saturation in PhD Studies Using Qualitative Interviews. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung / Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 11(3), Art. 8, http://nbnresolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0114-fqs100387

Onwuegbuzie, A., & Leech, N. L. (2007). Sampling designs in qualitative research: Making the sampling process more public. The Qualitative Report, 12(2), 238-254. Retrieved [Insert date], from http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR12-2/onwuegbuzie1.pdf